Read Caption

October 1, 2017, Las Vegas, Nevada | 58 killed, 546 injured

Firing from the 32nd floor of a hotel, a man armed with semiautomatic rifles, modified to fire faster, rained more than a thousand rounds on a music festival. Public mass shootings have become much more frequent since 2011.

The Science Behind Psychopaths and Extreme Altruists

Researchers have found that the way our brains are wired can affect how much empathy we feel toward others—a key measuring stick of good and evil.

View Images

December 14, 2012, Sandy Hook Elementary School | 27 killed, 2 injured

Six staff members and 20 children died in the Newtown, Connecticut, shooting. The assailant also killed his mother. The school was torn down and replaced. Nothing was saved from the old school, not even the flagpole.

View Images

June 7, 1998, Huff Creek Road, Jasper, Texas | 1 killed

White supremacists murdered James Byrd, Jr., an African American, by chaining him to a pickup truck and dragging him along this road. Evidence circles mark where pieces of Byrd’s body were found.

This story appears in the January 2018 issue of National Geographic magazine.

From the kitchen window of her mobile home in Auburn, Illinois, Ashley Aldridge had a clear view of the railroad crossing about a hundred yards away.

When the 19-year-old mother first saw the man in the wheelchair, she had just finished feeding lunch to her two children, ages one and three, and had moved on to washing dishes—one more in an endless string of chores. Looking up, Aldridge noticed that the wheelchair wasn’t moving. It was stuck between the tracks. The man was yelling for help as a motorcycle and two cars went by without stopping.

View Images
Ashley Aldridge


Facing down fear to help:
A young mother of two, Aldridge ran barefoot to a railroad crossing to rescue a man whose wheelchair was stuck on the tracks. She yanked him free just before a train would have hit them. “I don’t really think of myself as a hero, but my kids tell me all the time that I am,” she says. “I was just helping somebody, I guess is how I see it, because if that was my grandpa, I would hope somebody would help him too.”

Aldridge hurried out to ask a neighbor to watch her kids so she could go help. Then she heard the train horn and the clanging of the crossing gate as it came down, signaling that a train was on its way. She ran, barefoot, over a gravel path along the tracks. When she got to the man, the train was less than half a mile away, bearing down at about 80 miles an hour. Failing to dislodge the wheelchair, she wrapped her arms around the man’s chest from behind and tried to lift him, but couldn’t. As the train barreled toward them, she pulled with a mighty heave. She fell backward, yanking him out of the chair. Within seconds, the train smashed the wheelchair, carrying fragments of steel and plastic half a mile up the track.

The man Aldridge saved that afternoon in September 2015 was a complete stranger. Her unflinching determination to save him despite the threat to her own life sets her apart from many. Aldridge’s heroic rescue is an example of what scientists call extreme altruism—selfless acts to help those unrelated to oneself at the risk of grave personal harm. Not surprisingly, many of these heroes—such as Roi Klein, an Israeli army major who jumped on a live grenade to save his men—work in professions in which endangering one’s life to protect others is part of the job. But others are ordinary men and women—like Rick Best, Taliesin Namkai-Meche, and Micah Fletcher, who intervened to defend two young women, one wearing a hijab, from a man spewing anti-Muslim abuse at them on a commuter train in Portland, Oregon. All three were stabbed; only Fletcher survived.

View Images
Micah Fletcher


Intervening to help strangers:
Fletcher and two other men defended two women—one wearing a hijab—from a man spewing anti-Muslim abuse on a Portland commuter train. The assailant stabbed all three men. Two died, and Fletcher suffered a deep neck wound. He said he instinctively stepped in to help the women. Diagnosed as being on the autism spectrum as a child, he was bullied and beaten. “If you are truly a community, then everybody should be expected to stand up for one another,” he says.

Contrast these noble acts with the horrors that humans commit: murder, rape, kidnapping, torture. Consider the carnage perpetrated by the man who sprayed bullets from the 32nd floor of the Mandalay Bay hotel in Las Vegas, Nevada, in October at a country music festival. Three weeks later, officials put the casualty toll at 58 dead and 546 wounded. Or think about the chilling ruthlessness of a serial killer like Todd Kohlhepp, a real estate agent in South Carolina, who appears to have left clues about his murderous habit in bizarre online reviews for products, including a folding shovel: “Keep in car for when you have to hide the bodies.” In spite of how aberrant these horrors are, they occur often enough to remind us of a dark truth: Humans are capable of unspeakable cruelty.

Extreme altruists and psychopaths exemplify our best and worst instincts. On one end of the moral spectrum, sacrifice, generosity, and other ennobling traits that we recognize as good; on the other end, selfishness, violence, and destructive impulses that we see as evil. At the root of both types of behaviors, researchers say, is our evolutionary past. They hypothesize that humans—and many other species, to a lesser degree—evolved the desire to help one another because cooperation within large social groups was essential to survival. But because groups had to compete for resources, the willingness to maim and possibly kill opponents was also crucial. “We are the most social species on Earth, and we are also the most violent species on Earth,” says Jean Decety, a social neurologist at the University of Chicago. “We have two faces because these two faces were important to survival.”

For centuries the question of how good and evil originate and manifest in us was a matter of philosophical or religious debate. But in recent decades researchers have made significant advances toward understanding the science of what drives good and evil. Both seem to be linked to a key emotional trait: empathy, which is an intrinsic ability of the brain to experience how another person is feeling. Researchers have found that empathy is the kindling that fires compassion in our hearts, impelling us to help others in distress. Studies also have traced violent, psychopathic, and antisocial behaviors to a lack of empathy, which appears to stem from impaired neural circuits. These new insights are laying the foundation for training regimens and treatment programs that aim to enhance the brain’s empathic response.

View Images
Phineas Gage and the Frontal Lobe


Learning how the brain works:
A railroad construction foreman, Gage (top) survived an explosion in 1848 that drove an iron rod through his left frontal lobe. When he recovered, he was no longer friendly and respectful; he was uncaring and indifferent. A patient in Canada underwent a similar change when a benign tumor grew in his frontal lobe. After it was removed in 2016, his wife told the Sherbrooke University medical team, “Thank you for giving me back my husband.” His MRI scans (above) show before and after surgery. Cases like these help explain how brain structures guide social and moral behavior.

Researchers once thought young children had no concern for the well-being of others—a logical conclusion if you’ve seen a toddler’s tantrums. But recent findings show that babies feel empathy long before their first birthday. Maayan Davidov, a psychologist at Hebrew University of Jerusalem, and her colleagues have conducted some of these studies, analyzing the behavior of children as they witness somebody in distress—a crying child, an experimenter, or their own mother pretending to be hurt. Even before six months of age, many infants respond to such stimuli with facial expressions reflecting concern; some also exhibit caring gestures such as leaning forward and trying to communicate with the one in distress. In their first year, infants also show signs of trying to understand the suffering they’re seeing. Eighteen-month-olds often translate their empathy into such positive social behavior as giving a hug or a toy to comfort a hurt child.

That’s not true of all children, however. In a small minority, starting in the second year of life, researchers see what they call an “active disregard” of others. “When someone reported that someone had hurt themselves,” says Carolyn Zahn-Waxler, a researcher at the University of Wisconsin–Madison, “these children would kind of laugh at them or even kind of swipe at them and say, ‘You’re not hurt,’ or ‘You should be more careful’—saying it in a tone of voice that was judgmental.” Following these toddlers into adolescence, Zahn-Waxler and her colleague Soo Hyun Rhee, a psychologist at the University of Colorado Boulder, found they had a high likelihood of developing antisocial tendencies and getting into trouble.

Other studies have measured callousness and lack of emotional expression in adolescents using questions such as whether the subject feels remorseful upon doing something wrong. Those with high scores for “callous-unemotional” traits tend to have frequent and severe behavioral problems—showing extreme aggression in fights, for instance, or vandalizing property. Researchers have also found that some of these adolescents end up committing major crimes such as murder, rape, and violent robbery. Some are prone to becoming full-blown psychopaths as adults—individuals with cold, calculating hearts who wouldn’t flinch while perpetrating the most horrific acts imaginable. (Most psychopaths are men.)

Anatomy

of Empathy

Using scanning technology, scientists can

identify parts of the brain that are active when

we empathize with others. By combining those

results with other findings—from psycho-

logical evaluations to genetic testing—

researchers are beginning to determine which

biological and environmental factors reinforce

or corrode our capacity for empathy.

The Empathy Circuit

Highlighted areas have distinct roles

in how the brain responds to others.

TempOroparietal Junction

Imagining thoughts of others (right side only)

Superior Temporal Sulcus

Reading the eye expressions

and movements of others

 

Inferior Parietal Lobe

Mirroring people’s actions and emotions

 

Somatosensory Cortex

Viscerally reacting to others’ physical pain

 

ANTERIOR INSULA

Recognizing emotion, pain, and self-awareness

 

frontal operculum

Comprehending the intentions of others

 

Medial Prefrontal Cortex

Comparing one’s perspective with others’

Orbitofrontal cortex

Understanding social cues and physical pain

INFERIOR FRONTAL GYRUS

Registering facial emotion

 

Middle Cingulate Cortex

Recognizing pain

 

THE AMYGDALA

Extremely altruistic people have more

neural activity in their enlarged amygdalas—

a part of the brain associated with

learned emotional responses and the

processing of distressing stimuli.

THE PREFRONTAL CORTEX

A brain scan of a murderer who pleaded insanity

(above right) shows reduced activity in the

prefrontal cortex, a part of the brain that

restrains impulsive behavior. Abnormalities in

this area could predispose a person to violence.

Normal brain

Active

prefrontal cortex

Homicidal brain

Less active

prefrontal cortex

How the circuit is activated

Biological

Genetic variations enable some people to

recognize facial expressions better or

produce more of an enzyme related to

lower aggressiveness.

Psychological

A nurturing childhood can potentially

transform someone who is genetically

predisposed to lack empathy into a social,

nonviolent citizen.

Social

Watching a friend cry or hearing a dog

whimper, for example, can evoke empathy

and a desire to end what is perceived as

shared suffering.

How the circuit is suppressed

Biological

Some 70 percent of psychopathic traits are

genetic, according to one estimate. Brain

injury and prolonged exposure to stress

can also damage empathy circuitry.

Psychological

Temporary states such as drunkenness,

fear, or hunger can briefly reduce our

empathy responses, while childhood

trauma can have longer term effects.

Social

A widespread empathy shutdown can

occur when an entire group of people

conforms to an ideology of aggression or

superiority, such as during wartime.

The Empathy Spectrum

Empathy can be measured through empathy

quotient (EQ) tests. Questions aim to

determine the magnitude of one’s interest in

how others feel and think. Extreme altruists fall

at one end of the spectrum, those who totally

lack empathy at the other.

Distribution of

respondents

25%

20

15

10

5

75

65

55

45

35

25

15

5 and

below

Empathy quotient scores

More Empathic

Less Empathic

Extreme altruists

Highly empathic people, such as those who risk their

lives for strangers, are better able to recognize pain

or fear in others’ faces.

Professionals in the humanities

People such as musicians and historians typically

score higher on EQ tests.

Professionals in the sciences

Those who think in highly systematic ways often have

lower-than-average empathy responses.

Psychopaths

If they were honest, psychopaths might score

poorly—but they can recognize how and when

to feign empathy.

JASON TREAT AND RYAN T. WILLIAMS, NGM STAFF.

ART: BRYAN CHRISTIE SOURCES: SIMON BARON-COHEN,

DAVID M. GREENBERG, UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE;

BRAIN PET SCANS COURTESY ADRIAN RAINE,

UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA

Anatomy of Empathy

Normal brain

Homicidal brain

Active

prefrontal cortex

Less active

prefrontal cortex

Using scanning technology, scientists can identify parts of the brain

that are active when we empathize with others. By combining those

results with other findings—from psychological evaluations to

genetic testing—researchers are beginning to determine which

biological and environmental factors reinforce or corrode our

capacity for empathy.

THE PREFRONTAL CORTEX

A brain scan of a murderer who pleaded insanity

(above right) shows reduced activity in the

prefrontal cortex, a part of the brain that

restrains impulsive behavior. Abnormalities in

this area could predispose a person to violence.

Recognizing pain

 

The Empathy Circuit

Highlighted areas have distinct roles

in how the brain responds to others.

Viscerally reacting to others’ physical pain

 

Comparing one’s

perspective

with others’

Mirroring people’s actions and emotions

 

Reading the eye expressions

and movements of others

 

Understanding

social cues and

physical pain

Imagining thoughts of others

(right side only)

Recognizing emotion,

pain, and self-awareness

 

Registering facial emotion

 

THE AMYGDALA

Comprehending the

intentions of others

 

Extremely altruistic people have more

neural activity in their enlarged amygdalas—

a part of the brain associated with

learned emotional responses and the

processing of distressing stimuli.

Somatosensory Cortex

Superior Temporal Sulcus

INFERIOR FRONTAL GYRUS

ANTERIOR INSULA

Medial Prefrontal Cortex

Inferior Parietal Lobe

TempOroparietal Junction

frontal operculum

Middle Cingulate Cortex

Orbitofrontal cortex

How the circuit is activated

How the circuit is suppressed

Biological

Biological

Genetic variations enable some people to recognize

facial expressions better or produce more

of an enzyme related to lower aggressiveness.

Some 70 percent of psychopathic traits are genetic,

according to one estimate. Brain injury and prolonged

exposure to stress can also damage empathy circuitry.

Psychological

Psychological

A nurturing childhood can potentially transform

someone who is genetically predisposed to lack

empathy into a social, nonviolent citizen.

Temporary states such as drunkenness, fear, or

hunger can briefly reduce our empathy responses,

while childhood trauma can have longer term effects.

Social

Social

Watching a friend cry or hearing a dog whimper,

for example, can evoke empathy and a desire

to end what is perceived as shared suffering.

A widespread empathy shutdown can occur when

an entire group of people conforms to an ideology

of aggression or superiority, such as during wartime.

The Empathy Spectrum

Empathy can be measured through empathy quotient (EQ) tests.

Questions aim to determine the magnitude of one’s interest in how others

feel and think. Extreme altruists fall at one end of the spectrum, those

who totally lack empathy at the other.

Professionals in the humanities

Professionals in the sciences

Extreme altruists

People such as musicians and

historians typically score

higher on EQ tests.

Those who think in highly systematic

ways often have lower-than-average

empathy responses.

Highly empathic people, such as those

who risk their lives for strangers, are better

able to recognize pain or fear in others’ faces.

Distribution of

respondents

25%

20

Psychopaths

15

If they were honest, psychopaths might

score poorly—but they can recognize

how and when to feign empathy.

10

5

75

65

60

55

50

45

40

35

30

25

20

15

10

5 and

below

70

Empathy quotient scores

More Empathic

Less Empathic

JASON TREAT AND RYAN T. WILLIAMS, NGM STAFF. ART: BRYAN CHRISTIE

SOURCES: SIMON BARON-COHEN, DAVID M. GREENBERG, UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE;

BRAIN PET SCANS COURTESY ADRIAN RAINE, UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA

If the empathy deficit at the core of psychopathic behaviors can be traced all the way back to toddlerhood, does evil reside in the genes, coiled up like a serpent in the DNA, waiting to strike? The answer isn’t a categorical yes or no. As it is with many illnesses, both nature and nurture have a hand. Studies of twins have established that callous-unemotional traits displayed by some young children and adolescents arise to a substantial degree from genes they inherit. Yet in a study of 561 children born to mothers with a history of antisocial behaviors, researchers found that those living with adoptive families that provided a warm and nurturing environment were far less likely to exhibit callous-unemotional traits than those with adoptive families that were not as nurturing.

Children born with genes making it more likely that they will have difficulty empathizing are often unable to get a break. “You can imagine that if you have a child who doesn’t show affection in the same way as a typically developing child, doesn’t show empathy, that child will evoke very different reactions in the people around them—the parents, the teachers, the peers—than a child who’s more amenable, more empathetic,” says Essi Viding, a research psychologist at University College London. “And many of these children, of course, reside within their biological families, so they often have this double whammy of having parents who are perhaps less well equipped for many of the parenting tasks, are less good at empathizing, less good at regulating their own emotions.”

The firefighters tried desperately to save the six Philpott children from their burning house in Derby, England, in the early hours of May 11, 2012. But the heat and smoke were so intense that only one of the kids was alive when rescuers finally made their way upstairs where they had been sleeping. That boy, too, perished two days later in the hospital. The police suspected arson, based on evidence that the fire had been started by pouring gasoline through the door’s mail slot.

Derby residents raised money to help the children’s parents—Mick and Mairead Philpott—pay for a funeral. At a news conference to thank the community, Philpott was sobbing and dabbing his eyes with a tissue that remained curiously dry. Leaving the event, he collapsed, but Derbyshire’s assistant chief constable, walking behind, was struck by the unnaturalness of the behavior. Eighteen days later, the police arrested Philpott and his wife. Investigators determined that they had set fire to the house with an accomplice to frame Mick’s mistress. A court found all three guilty of manslaughter.

Philpott’s faking of grief and his lack of remorse are among the characteristics that define psychopaths, a category of individuals who have come to embody evil in the popular imagination. Psychopaths have utter disregard for the feelings of others, although they seem to learn to mimic emotions. “They really just have a complete inability to appreciate anything like empathy or guilt or remorse,” says Kent Kiehl, a neuroscientist at the Mind Research Network and the University of New Mexico who was drawn to studying psychopathy in part because he grew up in a neighborhood that was once home to the serial killer Ted Bundy. These are people who are “just extremely different than the rest of us.”

Kiehl has spent the past two decades exploring this difference by scanning the brains of prison inmates. (Nearly one in every five adult males in prison in the U.S. and Canada scores high in psychopathy, measured using a checklist of 20 criteria such as impulsivity and lack of remorse, compared with one out of every 150 in the general male population.)

Using an MRI scanner installed inside a tractor trailer, Kiehl and his colleagues have imaged more than 4,000 prison inmates since 2007, measuring the activity in their brains as well as the size of different brain regions.

Psychopathic criminals show reduced activity in their brain’s amygdala, a primary site of emotional processing, compared with non-psychopathic inmates when recalling emotionally charged words they were shown moments earlier, such as “misery” and “frown.” In a task designed to test moral decision-making, researchers ask inmates to rate the offensiveness of pictures flashed on a screen, such as a cross burning by the Ku Klux Klan or a face bloodied by a beating. Although the ratings by psychopathic offenders aren’t that different from those by non-psychopaths—they both recognize the moral violation in the pictures—psychopaths tend to show weaker activation in brain regions instrumental in moral reasoning.

Based on these and other, similar findings, Kiehl is convinced that psychopaths have impairments in a system of interconnected brain structures—including the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex—that help process emotions, make decisions, control impulses, and set goals. There is “basically about 5 to 7 percent less gray matter in those structures in individuals with high psychopathic traits compared to other inmates,” Kiehl says. The psychopath appears to compensate for this deficiency by using other parts of the brain to cognitively simulate what really belongs in the realm of emotion. “That is, the psychopath must think about right and wrong while the rest of us feel it,” Kiehl wrote in a paper he co-authored in 2011.

When Abigail Marsh, a psychologist at Georgetown University, was 19, her car skidded on a bridge after she swerved to avoid hitting a dog. The vehicle spun out of control and finally came to a stop in the fast lane, facing oncoming traffic. Marsh couldn’t get the engine to start and was too afraid to get out, with cars and trucks rushing past the vehicle. A man pulled over, ran across the highway, and helped start the car. “He took an enormous risk running across the freeway. There’s no possible explanation for it other than he just wanted to help,” Marsh says. “How can anybody be moved to do something like that?”

Marsh kept turning that question over in her head. Not long after she began working at Georgetown, she wondered if the altruism shown by the driver on the bridge wasn’t in some ways the polar opposite of psychopathy. She began looking for a group of exceptionally kind individuals to study and decided that altruistic kidney donors would make ideal subjects. These are people who’ve chosen to donate a kidney to a stranger, sometimes even incurring financial costs, yet receive no compensation in return.

Marsh and her colleagues brought 19 donors in from around the country for the study. The researchers showed each one a series of black-and-white photographs of facial expressions, some fearful, some angry, and others neutral, while their brains were scanned using an MRI machine to map both activity and structure.

View Images
Anthony Chavez


Guiding others to safety:
When Chavez (above) saw a woman next to him covered in blood at a Las Vegas music festival, he realized he and his girlfriend, Deianara Torres (at right), were under fire. He helped her escape to safety over a crowd barrier near the stage, then saw another concertgoer they’d met earlier. “We locked eyes, and it was complete fear,” he says. “I knew I had to stay there and help her.” He aided her and several others paralyzed by shock, boosting them over the fence.

View Images
William Ramirez


Getting in harm’s way:
On his way to his job washing boats, Ramirez (above) rescued a Miami police officer from a barrage of bullets fired by a man with an assault rifle. He drove his van in between the officer and the shooter, opened a door so the officer could jump in, and sped away to safety. “I could not just go by, go to work without doing anything. The police risk their lives for people all the time,” he told the Sun Sentinel newspaper.

When looking at fearful faces, donors showed a greater response in their right amygdala than a control group. Separately, the researchers found that their right amygdalas were, on average, 8 percent larger than those of the control group. Similar studies done previously on psychopathic subjects had found the opposite: The amygdalas in psychopathic brains are smaller and activated less than those in controls while reacting to frightened faces.

“Fearful expressions elicit concern and caring. If you’re not responsive to that expression, you’re unlikely to experience concern for other people,” Marsh explains. “And altruistic kidney donors just seem to be very sensitive to other people’s distress, with fear being the most acute kind of distress—maybe in part because their amygdalas are larger than average.”

The majority of people in the world are neither extreme altruists nor psychopaths, and most individuals in any society do not ordinarily commit violent acts against one another. And yet, there are genocides—organized mass killings that require the complicity and passivity of large numbers of people. Time and again, social groups organized along ethnic, national, racial, and religious lines have savaged other groups. Nazi Germany’s gas chambers extinguished millions of Jews, the Communist Khmer Rouge slaughtered fellow Cambodians in the killing fields, Hutu extremists in Rwanda wielding machetes slaughtered several hundred thousand Tutsis and moderate Hutus, and Islamic State terrorists massacred Iraq’s Yazidis—virtually every part of the world appears to have suffered through a genocide. Events such as these provide ghastly evidence that evil can hold entire communities in its grip.

How the voice of conscience is rendered inconsequential to foot soldiers of a genocide can be partly understood through the prism of the well-known experiments conducted in the 1960s by the psychologist Stanley Milgram at Yale University. In those studies, subjects were asked to deliver electric shocks to a person in another room for failing to answer questions correctly, increasing the voltage with every wrong answer. At the prodding of a person in a lab coat who played the role of an experimenter, the subjects often dialed up the shocks to dangerously high voltage levels. The shocks weren’t real and the cries of pain heard by the subjects were prerecorded, but the subjects only found that out afterward. The studies demonstrated what Milgram described as “the extreme willingness of adults to go to almost any lengths on the command of an authority.”

Gregory Stanton, a former U.S. State Department official and founder of Genocide Watch, a nonprofit that works to prevent mass murder, has identified the stages that can cause otherwise decent people to commit murder. It starts when demagogic leaders define a target group as “the other” and claim it is a threat to the interests of supporters. Discrimination follows, and soon the leaders characterize their targets as subhuman, eroding the in-group’s empathy for “the other.”

Next, society becomes polarized. “Those planning the genocide say, ‘You are either with us or against us,’ ” says Stanton. This is followed by a phase of preparation, with the architects of the genocide drawing up death lists, stocking weapons, and planning how the rank and file are to execute the killings. Members of the out-group are sometimes forced to move into ghettos or concentration camps. Then the massacres begin.

Many of the perpetrators remain untouched by remorse, not because they are incapable of feeling it—as is the case with psychopathic killers—but because they find ways to rationalize the killings. James Waller, a genocide scholar at Keene State College in New Hampshire, says he got a glimpse of this “incredible capacity of the human mind to make sense of and to justify the worst of actions” when he interviewed dozens of Hutu men convicted or accused of committing atrocities during the Rwandan genocide. Some of them had hacked children, even those they personally knew, to death. Their rationale, according to Waller, was: “If I didn’t do this, those children would have grown up to come back to kill me. This was something that was a necessity for my people to be safe, for my people to survive.”

Our capacity to empathize and channel that into compassion may be innate, but it is not immutable. Neither is the tendency to develop psychopathic and antisocial personalities so fixed in childhood as to be unchangeable. In recent years researchers have shown the feasibility of nipping evil in the bud as well as strengthening our positive social instincts.

The possibility of preventing violent teenage boys from hardening into lifelong criminals has been put to the test at the Mendota Juvenile Treatment Center in Wisconsin, a facility that houses serious offenders but is run more as a psychiatric unit than as a prison. The adolescents referred to the center come in with already long criminal histories—teenagers who are a threat to others. “These are folks who essentially have dropped out of the human race—they don’t have any connection to anyone, and they are in a real antagonistic posture with everybody,” says Michael Caldwell, a senior staff psychologist.

The center attempts to build a connection with the kids despite their aggressive and antisocial behaviors. Even when an inmate hurls feces or sprays urine at staff members—a common occurrence at many correctional institutions—the staff members keep treating the offender humanely. The kids are scored on a set of behavior rating scales every day. If they do well, they earn certain privileges the following day, such as a chance to play video games. If they score badly, say, by getting into a fight, they lose privileges. The focus is not on punishing bad behavior but on rewarding good conduct. That’s different from most correctional institutions. Over time the kids start to behave better, says Greg Van Rybroek, the center’s director. Their callous-unemotional traits diminish. Their improved ability to manage their emotions and control their violent impulses seems to endure beyond the walls of Mendota. Adolescents treated in the program have committed far fewer and less violent offenses between two and six years after release than those treated elsewhere, the center’s studies have found. “We don’t have any magic,” Van Rybroek says, “but we’ve actually created a system that considers the world from the youth’s point of view and tries to break it down in a fair and consistent manner.”

During the past decade researchers have discovered that our social brain is plastic, even in adulthood, and that we can be trained to be more kind and generous. Tania Singer, a social neuroscientist at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences in Leipzig, Germany, has pioneered studies demonstrating this.

View Images
Sue Klebold and Coni Sanders


Sharing sorrow:
Klebold (at left) is the mother of Dylan Klebold, one of two teenagers who carried out the 1999 shooting at Colorado’s Columbine High School. Klebold wrote about her experience in A Mother’s Reckoning and donates any profits to mental health charities. She has become a mother figure to Sanders (at right), whose father was killed in the massacre. For years Sanders was angry with Klebold for raising a son who became a killer. But her struggle to raise her own teenagers helped her understand Klebold. “If anyone’s pain is greater than my own, it’s hers,” she says. The women, here at a memorial for the victims in Olinger Chapel Hill cemetery, now share a strong bond.

Empathy and compassion use different networks in the brain, Singer and her colleagues found. Both can lead to positive social behavior, but the brain’s empathic response to seeing another person suffer can sometimes lead to empathic distress—a negative reaction that makes the onlooker want to turn away from the sufferer to preserve his or her own sense of well-being.

To enhance compassion, which combines awareness of another’s distress with the desire to alleviate it, Singer and her colleagues have tested the effects of various training exercises. A prominent exercise, derived from Buddhist traditions, involves having subjects meditate on a loved one—a parent or a child, for example—directing warmth and kindness toward that individual and gradually extending those same feelings toward acquaintances, strangers, and even enemies, in an ever widening circle of love. Singer’s group has shown that subjects who trained in this form of loving-kindness meditation even for a few days had a more compassionate response—as measured by the activation of certain brain circuits—than untrained subjects, when watching short film clips of people suffering emotional distress.

In another study, Singer and her colleagues tested the effects of compassion training on helpfulness by using a computer game in which subjects guide a virtual character on a computer screen through a maze to a treasure chest, opening gates along the way. They can also choose to open gates for another character wandering about, looking for treasure. The researchers found that subjects who underwent compassion training were more helpful than those in a control group toward the other character—the equivalent of a stranger.

That we might be able to mold our brains to be more altruistic is an ennobling prospect for society. One way to bring that future closer, Singer believes, would be to include compassion training in schools. The result could be a more benevolent world, populated by people like Ashley Aldridge, in which reflexive kindness loses its extraordinariness and becomes a defining trait of humanity.

Yudhijit Bhattacharjee is a contributing writer for National Geographic. Lynn Johnson has photographed many features for the magazine.